The Missouri Reader Vol. 38, Issue 2 | Page 42

42

Yannicopoulou (2004) examined visual literacy in school children as it relates to the reading of comics/graphic novel texts. 350 pre-school children “were personally interviewed regarding four variables of visual conventions common not only in comics but also in many multimodal materials preschool-ers encounter” (Yannicopoulou, 2004, p. 175). Overall, across all four categories of volume, words, language, and emotions depicted in the panels for identification, the children scored well over 70 percent correct indicating that the preschoolers possessed both a high degree of familiarity with the comic book conventions and the visual literacy required to derive intended meaning from texts which they could not read in printed language. Yannicopoulou (2004) concludes that:

Traditional teachers who limit their

understanding of literacy to decoding the

signs of the written language, must

recognize that ‘reading’ had been expanded

to reader-viewers who exploit verbal and

visual hints in the texts they read. The

challenge of multimodality should change

the concept of what ‘reading’ is and how it is

taught in schools. (p. 189)

Dallacqua (2012) conducted her research with 70 fifth graders in a small, Midwestern parochial school. She was interested in the gutters, or empty spaces, that generally exist between panels of comics as elucidated by McCloud (1993). She was supported in her thinking by Rosenblatt’s (1982) reader-response theory. Dallacqua (2012) found “these literal gaps, or gutters, in graphic novels provided ample opportunities for reader to engage and contribute to the storytelling” (p. 370). She credits Sipe and Brightman (2009) for work with children and picture books grounded in reader-response theory. According to Dallacqua (2012) this work “led to the theory that allowing young readers to fill in the gaps at page breaks of picture books may enrich meaning-making and invite high-level inferencing skills” (p. 369). Dallacqua determined that her readers were able to negotiate the multimodal comic texts and distinguish many literary devices common to the traditional print texts that they had studied previously in school. Point of view, allusion, symbolism, metaphor, mood, flashback and foreshadowing were all literary elements the students were able to identify and explain in relation to the specific occurrences in the graphic novels under study.

Connors (2013) “sought to identify the semiotic resources six high school students drew upon as they read four graphic novels in the context of a voluntary after-school reading group” (p. 34). Connors used reader response journals, transcripts of the reading group discussions, and transcripts of individual member interviews to build his data base using categories of design established by the New London Group (1996). Connors (2013) searched for recurring patterns and deter-mined that all six participants dealt with ele-ments of the comics’ multimodal text “as meaning-making resources available to them in the visual design of the graphic novels…” and that “…in interacting with the graphic novel’s linguistic design they tended to read in a manner that called to mind Rosenblatt’s (1978) theory of transactional reading" (Connors, 2013, p. 38). Connors concludes with a challenge to educators:

To revisit their definitions of literacy and

ask whether they are willing to extend that

term to include transactions with sign

systems beyond written language. The

implications of doing so are profound, not

least because it raises the question of

whether schools are currently preparing

students to read and write in a broad sense.

(Connors, 2013, p. 49)

"...her readers were able to

negotiate the multimodal

comic texts and distinguish

many literary devices common

to the traditional print texts

that they had studied

previously in school."