SotA Anthology 2015-16 | Page 31

ENGL276 McEnery himself points out the issues with this methodology when trying to analyse particular parts of his research into swearing and gender; it cannot be confirmed what the gender of the hearer is, and therefore one cannot analyse accurately if this affects the speaker’s language use. He talks of how this would be something of interest to look into, however “the inability to annotate hearer information in the corpus precludes the exploration of this’ (2006, p.38). Hence a corpus-based approach to bad language use has its advantages and limitations. What we can discover from McEnery’s work, however, is the scale of offensiveness of both men and women’s bad language. In his study he includes a table in which various expletives are categorised into the following: very mild, mild, moderate, strong, very strong (McEnery, 2006, p.36). This will be discussed later in more detail relating to my own research, but for McEnery’s work this is ideal in exploring the general use of swearing of females and males. By using a scale of offence to measure their bad language use, one can identify if one gender in general uses more offensive BLWs than the other, and the implication of this in the wider context of sociolinguistics. McEnery’s work covers a range of different factors as noted previously, and therefore other works on gender and language use need to be taken into account. Two sociolinguists in the field of language and gender are Jennifer Coates and Robin Lakoff - both with opposing views. One of the instigators in large studies on language and gender is Lakoff, who talks in her work of the oppression of women and the dominance of men not just in society, but in their language use as well (Lakoff 1975/2004). Lakoff’s work on gender and language would be an approach associated with either deficit or dominance. This deficit approach claims that there is a certain deficit within female or ‘feminine’ language, as Lakoff talks of, that reinforces their social standing. Dominance is slightly different, claiming that in a mixed-sex setting, males are expected to interrupt or dominate, more than females. This can relate back to McEnery, when he claims that women use weaker word forms than men; a similar stance to Lakoff in her talk of female’s 31 ‘meaningless particles’. With approaches such as these, swearing becomes not just a feature of everyday interaction, but emphasises the different societal roles of males and females. Lakoff goes one step further in her work to claim that the ‘stronger’ exp letives are ‘reserved’ for men, and the weaker ones for women (1975/2004, p.44). Again, referring to the females’ language use as a weaker lexical choice suggests there is some sort of deficit or subordination within the female vocabulary. The problem with analysing data exclusively through the lens of the dominance approach is that it can have a tendency to take on a folk-like view on language and can become more prescriptive than descriptive when there is not sufficient evidence, a common criticism of Lakoff’s work (Coates 2004). It is at this point that the difference approach (Zimmerman and West 1975) has a crucial role. Coates uses this theory as a means to analyse her own data in her works Women, Men and Language (2004) and Women, Men, and Everyday Talk (2013). The difference approach does not claim that one gender’s language is subordinate to the other, but identifies that there are differences between the language used by males and females. We can see this most clearly in Coates’s work on allfemale interaction (1989) and all-male interaction (2013); female interaction is