Re: Spring 2014 | Page 88

behaviour (for example a sit) is repeated a few times for a click and then a treat, the owner then begins to train the animal a cue (like the word “sit”). The trainer then only marks and rewards the behaviour if the animal does it after the cue has been given. Once a behaviour is on cue the trainer can stop using the clicker and just reward the animal with a treat. The method has been used to teach dogs like Pugsley to dance, clams to clap their shells and goldfish to swim through hoops. The idea is that if it eats, it can be clicker trained. The method was a more positive way of thinking - helping dogs to learn what we want rather than punishing what we don’t want. It implied, much to the upset of traditional ‘dominance’ trainers - that dogs wanted to partake in desirable behaviours but just didn’t know how. This seemed to also refute the idea that dogs behaved because they wanted to challenge their owners...and science also backed this up... wolves in the living room... The mythology which had been used to explain our domestic dogs’ behaviour in the past, also changed around the same time. In the late 60s, we believed that because dogs and wolves were the same species, they must behave the same too. A guy called Dr Mech wrote a book called The Wolf: Ecology and Behaviour of an endangered species and in it he noted his observations on captive wolf packs. He had theorised that dogs, like wolves, are pack animals and have a natural linear hierarchy (i.e. an “Alfa” at the top who leads the pack). He suggested that dogs also dominant each other - fighting for higher positions. In the 60s this opinion became very popular amongst dog trainers and handlers. A good dog owner would dominate their dog and show that they were Alpha by behaving the way Mech had witnessed his “Alfa” wolves behave. Pinning dogs down and trying to get them to ‘submit’ was the only way to establish a good pecking order in the house. Challenging dogs were thus referred to as ‘dominant dogs’ and all behaviour from trying to go through the door first; to wanting to sit on the sofa - was explained in the context of the dog attempting to dominate the owner and establish a higher position in the pack.. However, Mech himself points out, in his 2008 article Whatever Happened to the Term Alfa Wolf? that, having then studied wild wolves in natural pack formation behaviour was far more social than he originally thought. Packs behaved more like family units, based on cooperation as opposed to conflict. He explained that conflict would displace families and leave the wolf packs in more vulnerable positions and that guiding their young lead to a more secure genetic future and so benefited all. 86 Biologist Dr Ray Coppinger also challenged this household meme, supposing that it was actually quite likely that dogs domesticated themselves and changed their behaviour patterns to adapt to settlements. The early view that humans kept wolves as pets and bred the easiest ones to handle seemed not only hard to imagine (when catching and containing wild wolves it so impossibly hard - let alone selectively breeding them for domestication), but also unlikely. He described ancient wolves that learnt themselves that it was easier to live off human waste than to hunt - with the tamest dogs that could go closest to the humans thriving and thus producing more offspring. He explained that it was then much more useful to study free roaming village dogs in developing countries, than wolves - if we wanted to know how dogs behaved in a natural state. These free roaming village dogs didn’t form packs. They compete against each other for resources and because they rarely hunt have smaller brains and less powerful jaws and teeth (just like domestic dogs). Dominance is dying... These important changes in understanding domestic dog origin as well as how dogs learn has bred a new type of dog training practice. A practice which is built on theories and experiments and not upon myth. Actually much of the practice is based on logic too. When we break down a dog’s behaviour we see that they are perfectly functional creatures. Renowned behaviourist Dr Susan Friedman explains: “There is an inherent connection between an animal’s behaviour and the environment in which it behaves. Science confirms that behaviour doesn’t spray out of animals willy-nilly like water from a leaky showerhead: Animals behave for a reason, to affect the environment in some way.” Those ‘dominant dogs’ who want to sleep on the sofa as a way of symbolising a challenge to you - their leader - are perhaps smarter than we first thought? When we look at the