Plaastoe! Julie 2017 Uitgawe 17 | Page 45

multicoloured balloons, umbrellas, plastic tarps, pylons or rattles.  This makes the horse much more capable of handling novel events calmly. If desensitization is not done correctly, it has the potential to make a horse more fearful and sensitive, for instance by removing an alarming stimulus too soon. Pavlovian conditioning Pavlovian conditioning does not only work for dogs. A horse also learns effortlessly when initially unimportant stimuli or events are regularly coupled with stimuli that initiate some sort of response. This form of conditioning is used by a trainer when he links a word with a behaviour. The word ‘trot’ would mean nothing to an untrained horse, but if this is regularly paired with the flick of a whip (associated with invoking a flight response or pain) immediately before the horse changes gait on an upward transition, the horse soon learns to respond to th e verbal cue, without the use of a whip. The use of the word ‘good’ when correct behaviour is carried out, followed by a food treat, informs the horse that a food reinforcer is forthcoming. To many a veterinarian’s dismay, horses often learn that the sight of a syringe is associated with pain. The unconditioned horse will then attempt to escape if he has not been trained to accept such handling.  Stabled horses will learn to associate certain sounds with feeding, for example doors opening, hay being poured into a bucket, or will respond to visual cues, such as the arrival of the caretaker. They might then display unwelcome anticipatory behaviours, such as vocalizing, pawing or kicking stall doors. When reinforced, these may become conditioned behaviours.  For instance, when a horse, upon hearing grain being poured, starts kicking his door, a person may feed the horse hurriedly in an attempt to get him to settle down. This positively reinforces the undesirable behaviour and if this scenario repeats itself, the horse has effectively trained the human to feed him quickly. he desires, or negative when something is removed. Both types strengthen the relationship between stimulus and sought-after response, so that when the same stimulus is given again, there is an increased chance that the horse will repeat the action. In the early stages of learning the meaning of a new stimulus, the horse may respond randomly and hit upon the desired response by accident. Reinforcement of the response at the correct moment will cause the animal to repeat the behaviour. Horses excel at this type of learning, especially when positive reinforcement is given. Negative reinforcement Unfortunately, negative reinforcement is most often used in training. Horses are typically trained to perform actions to avoid something aversive. Under saddle they move forward when leg pressure is applied by the rider; on the ground, they yield their hindquarters when pressure is applied to the flank; they back up when pressure is applied to the bridge of their nose; and they enter a trailer to avoid whips, or pressure from ropes. A good trainer refines this by reinforcing the correct and ignoring the incorrect. In this way only the slightest pressure will eventually produce the desired response, making the partnership between horse and human appear effortless. Research has been done on the use of positive reinforcement principles to facilitate trailer loading of horses. It is not uncommon for a horse to be reluctant to be loaded onto a trailer. This can be very time consuming and hazardous for horse and handler. A horse may resist by rearing, pulling back, pawing, kicking or even falling over. If the owner gives up and fails to load the horse, such behaviour is reinforced. Traditional loading methods are based on negative reinforcement, often also accompanied by some punishment for an uncooperative horse. Researchers, Ferguson and Rosales-Rui found that, with positive reinforcement and target training, horses learned to load willingly and improper behaviours ceased. These effects also generalized to other novel situations. Horses that have learned to load through positive reinforcement do so eagerly, sometimes requiring only a verbal or visual signal, even when unrestrained. Positive reinforcement can also be targeted at lifting feet for hoof care, groundwork, grooming and veterinary handling. Discrimination learning Discrimination learning in horses has been researched since the 1930s. In discrimi­ nation tasks, horses must learn that one stimulus, and not another, will result in positive reinforcement. That particular stimulus then begins to control behaviour, so that the horse acts in a specific manner in the presence of one stimulus, but not the other. In one study a specific horse learnt 20 pairs of discriminations. This horse also ‘learnt to learn’ by using a general pattern to more easily solved subsequent tests. After 6 months, he had also retained 77.5% of Operant conditioning Operant conditioning, in contrast to classical or Pavlovian conditioning, occurs when an animal manipulates its environment to obtain reinforcement. This can be positive when he receives something Plaastoe! July 2017 43