Obiter Dicta Issue 9 - January 19, 2015 | Page 5

OPINION Monday, January 19, 2015   5 Was it Worth it? A Critical Review of the Effectiveness of CIA Torture tracey leigh dowdeswell › contributor W i t h t h e r e c e n t release of the U.S. Senate Select Committee on Intelligence’s report on torture committed by the Central Intelligence Agency, there has been a renewed debate on the effectiveness of torture in uncovering actionable intelligence. Torture is depicted as producing useful information previously unknown to the interrogator, and this must then be balanced against the damage done to our democratic values by engaging in an inherently illiberal practice. One common example is the “ticking time-bomb scenario,” a thought experiment in which it is imagined that a suspect has direct knowledge of a bomb that will kill in large numbers, and who is reluctant to disclose its location. Torture, it is argued, is a morally justified solution in such a scenario, one that will uncover information about future attacks and save lives. This view has been advanced by such leading American jurists as Alan Dershowitz and Richard Posner. Costanzo and Gerrity describe the torture narrative promoted by Dershowitz as one that reconfigures the torturer as a “principled, heroic figure who reluctantly uses torture to save lives,” while the government seeks to portray torture as a regulated, controlled precision process that is only used as a last resort in cases of extremity. An alternative way to characterize torture is to view it as part of a broader system of social control. The real value of torture lies not in its ability to uncover factually accurate information from recalcitrant individuals, but in its ability to construct an image of our enemies as morally unworthy, as lacking in basic humanity and human motivations, and therefore as outside the scope of morality and justice. Einolf, in his historical review of the use of torture, has found that torture is primarily used against those who are not full members of society, such as racial and ethnic minorities, slaves, and prisoners of war, and that torture is generally only used when the state itself is perceived to be under threat. For example, the use of torture in the Middle Ages against those suspected of heresy and witchcraft was highly successful in supporting this worldview by extracting confessions and convincing those tortured to name other guilty parties; this created an ever-expanding circle of new confessions and new perpetrators to interrogate. Langbein states that torture sustained a belief in the dangers of witchcraft, despite the fact that the falsity of the information produced through torture was widely recognized and commented upon. Similarly, the third century A.D. Roman jurist Ulpian recognized that information obtained under torture was not reliable, even as the practice was extended from slaves to the lower classes in general. In modern times, the Khmer Rouge of Cambodia used torture to provide substantial false evidence of a widespread conspiracy being perpetrated by the (non-existent) Kampuchean Worker’s Party. In this view, the real value of torture lies in its ability to reconstruct and reinforce the state’s public narrative of the essential goodness of the established social order, and to promote the legitimacy of its brutal suppression of those who would threaten this. Research in the social sciences supports this view of torture, and several studies have found that the more coercive the interrogation, the less accurate and truthful is the information obtained from detainees. Janeff-Bulman states that torture causes dissociation, leading detainees to become unwilling or unable to cooperate, and decreases the chances that they will be able to provide reliable information. Studies of U.S. POWs who were tortured in the Korean War (19501953) found that violent methods of interrogation had been less effective in eliciting information from the soldiers than were non-violent methods. Costanzo and G er r it y rev iew numerous studies that show that coercive interrogation techniques have produced false confessions in a surprisingly large number of cases. The stronger the level of coercion used, the greater the likelihood that false confessions will result; similarly, the longer the interrogation continues, the greater is the probability that any confessions produced will be false. A recent study by Goodman-Delahunty et al. of interrogations of high value detainees concluded that the use of coercive tactics, and physical coercion in particular, were negatively correlated with detainee cooperation, the disclosure of information, and the speed of disclosure. The Senate Report concerning the CIA’s enhanced interrogation program also supports this second view of torture, and contradicts the thesis that torture provided objectively correct inf ܛX][ۈ]