Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 51-3 | Page 76

222 F.-C. Chen et al. Our second hypothesis, that finger soaking enhances sensitivity to LT, was also confirmed, since the present data show, for the first time, that children with or without DCD exhibited increased sensitivity to LT after their fingers had been soaked in surfactant-water solution. These results corroborate those of Verrillo et al., who found that immersing fingers or hands in a surfactant- water solution enhances the sensitivity of touch-related perception in young adults (14). Nevertheless, our data were insufficient to illustrate the mechanism behind the changes in sensitivity to LT resulting from finger soaking. We propose that the effects of finger soaking on increasing sensitivity to LT could be caused by modifi- cations in mechanical characteristics, i.e., skin softness, as a result of increased hydration of the corneal layers of the fingers (27–29). Further studies measuring hydration and/or hardness of the fingertip will help to identify the factors responsible for the effects of surfactant-water immersion on sensitivity to LT in children with or with­ out DCD. Briefly, this study demonstrates that finger soaking can effectively increase sensitivity to LT in the fingertips of children in both DCD and TDC groups. Our third hypothesis, that finger soaking would modulate the body sway response during LT in both groups, was partially confirmed. The data revealed the novel finding that, after the fingertips had been soaked in surfactant-water solution, the stabilizing effects of LT were augmented only in the DCD group. Regar- ding the mechanism of how variations in sensitivity to LT may affect the control of body sway, Jeka et al. (8) and Balden et al. (9) proposed that light fingertip contact on an external reference location could pro- vide additional and useful somatosensory informa- tion about body motion, thus enhancing individuals’ postural stability. Indeed, during maintenance of LT, the cutaneous receptor could not only detect skin stretching and force at the contact location, but also receive proprioceptive signals of the in-contact upper extremity and provide further cues concerning body sway direction and amplitude regarding the reference location (12). In addition, Kouzaki & Masani (30) found the stabilizing effects of LT were mitigated when removing sensory cues elicited from the fingertip by tourniquet ischaemia. A more recent study indicated that individuals with a higher level of sensitivity to LT were capable of reducing their body sway to a greater extent when executing a LT compared with those with lower level sensitivity to LT (21). Finally, the present study indicated that, when the sensitivity to LT of children with DCD increased, the stabilizing effects of LT on body sway were enhanced. Given that a lower-threshold (higher-sensitivity) sensory receptor is easier to trigger via peripheral stimuli, children with www.medicaljournals.se/jrm DCD should have a greater, or at least equal, number of afferent signals arising from LT leading to the CNS in the LTAS condition compared with the LT condition. Combining these considerations, it seems reasonable to suggest that extra sensory information arising from sensitivity-enhanced touch receptors in the fingertips facilitates the stabilizing effects of LT. Interestingly and unexpectedly, the results of the current study showed that, while finger soaking in- creased sensitivity to LT in both groups, body sway was reduced only in the DCD population. We surmised that this was simply due to a floor effect of body sway in TDC. As they already have low amplitude of body sway, TDC may have less “space” to decrease their own body sway, therefore leading to an interaction effect in which, after finger soaking, children with DCD exhibited relatively more evident changes/de- creases regarding body sway while performing a LT compared with TDC. Bair et al.’s study indicated that children with DCD tended to be less effective in using cues from LT to reduce body sway compared with TDC (6). Based on the present results, it appears that immersion in surfactant-water solution can potentially benefit children with DCD by promoting sensitivity to LT, thus in turn compensating for smaller effects of LT (reducing body sway). A rehabilitation programme often involves practicing while therapists alter the availability of sensory information. Therefore, we suggest that therapists can employ finger soaking as a rehabilitation strategy to immediately augment sensory inputs obtained through LT, as well as to enhance the effects of LT on reducing body sway. Further research is needed to determine how long the effects of finger soaking persist, and whether the use of finger soaking applied in rehabilitation routines induces long-lasting improvements in sensitivity to LT and the effects of LT in children with DCD. The strengths of this study were the implementation of an evidence-based intervention (finger soaking in surfactant-water solution) (14) and employing a well- established study design that requires arm posture be kept the same across experimental conditions to avoid the confounding effects of different arm configurations (21, 22). A limitation of the current study was that the motion- tracking device used was equipped with only 4 chan- nels or sensors, thus it was not feasible to record the movement of the non-dominant limbs (finger, palm, forearm, and upper arm). Therefore, it is not known whether non-dominant limb movement differed bet- ween groups and/or differed among experimental conditions, and it is uncertain whether non-dominant limb movements substantially influenced the results.