Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 51-3 | Page 72

218 F.-C. Chen et al. It is notable that, in both Bair et al.’s and Chen & Tsai’s studies, arm configurations differed between ex- perimental conditions (6, 7). In the NT condition, both arms were held alongside the body. In the LT condition, however, the dominant elbow was maintained at a cer- tain angle of flexion (90° or 135°) when making contact with a bar or plate using a fingertip. An earlier study has documented that merely changing the orientation of the arm significantly adjusts proprioceptive input, which affects body sway, demonstrating that body kinematics may be indirectly affected by the posture of the upper extremity (12). Therefore, the stabilizing effects of LT may be confounded by variations in arm configurations between the NT and LT conditions. It is impossible to identify the “true” effects of LT on adapting body sway if the confounding impact of arm configuration is not taken into account and well controlled. It should also be noted, however, that previous stu- dies have also shown significantly smaller effects of LT on decreasing body sway in children with DCD than in TDC (6, 7). In other words, the use of LT reduced the amplitude of body sway less in children with DCD than in TDC. A potential explanation may involve somato- sensory deficits at the peripheral level in children with DCD. That is to say, children with DCD may be less sensitive (i.e. have a higher threshold) to LT, which can result in a lower level of feedback cues/information to the CNS, thus weakening the impact of the effects of LT on postural stability. Unfortunately, sensitivity to LT has not been measured in previous studies. To investigate whether children with DCD are less sensitive to LT, this study measured sensitivity to LT on the fingertip. Furthermore, to determine whether sensitivity to LT is involved in the smaller effects of LT on reducing body sway, this study attempted to change/ increase sensitivity to LT and measure its impact on the effects of LT. A study comparing sensitivity to LT before and after soaking the hands in water, showed that sensitivity to LT on the fingers was significantly increased after 30 min of immersion (13). A study by Verrillo et al. (14) further noted that after only 5 min of immersion in a 2% solution of sodium dodecyl sulphate, sensitivity to touch-related perception, i.e. roughness and texture, was significantly enhanced, and that this effect persisted for up to 30 min. According to Verrillo et al.’s study (14) and our pilot experiment, soaking in a surfactant-water solution appears to be an effective way to achieve a marked and longer-lasting increase in sensitivity to LT. The aims of this study were to compare sensitivity to LT in children with DCD and TDC, and to examine how changes/increases in sensitivity to LT impact on the ef- fects of LT on reducing body sway in both groups, while controlling for the possible confounding effects of arm www.medicaljournals.se/jrm configuration. The 3 research questions addressed were: (i) Are children with DCD less sensitive to LT compared with controls? (ii) Does immersion in surfactant-water solution alter sensitivity to LT in children with or wit- hout DCD? (iii) Are the effects of LT on reducing body sway in children with or without DCD altered by im- mersing their fingertips in surfactant-water solution? It was hypothesized: (i) that children with DCD would exhibit a lower level of sensitivity to LT compared with controls; (ii) that after immersing the fingertip in surfactant-water solution, children with DCD and their counterparts would have increased sensitivity to LT; and (iii) enhanced effects of LT on decreasing body sway. METHODS Prior to entering this study, the experimental procedure was fully explained and all participants and their legal guardians signed informed consent forms. This study was approved by the ethics committee of Antai Memorial Hospital, and performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki 1975. Participants A convenience sample of 52 children aged 11–12 years, including 26 children with DCD (12 boys and 14 girls) and 26 TDC (11 boys and 15 girls), was recruited from 3 urban elementary schools in Kaohsiung City, Taiwan. Children with DCD all scored at or below the 5th percentile from the 2 nd edition of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2) (15). Following Chen et al.’s study (7), the TDC group had a MABC-2 score above the 50 th percentile. No participants had intelligent impairments (as assessed by the score from the 2 nd edition of Kaufmann Brief Intelligence Test > 80) (16), behavioural symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (evaluated according to a score < 70 on Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale) (17), or recent injuries/orthopaedic conditions that might affect postural control capacities (as confirmed by parental reports). All participants were strongly right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory in which a –100 score denotes a complete left-handed preference, and a +100 score denotes a complete right-handed (18). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Table I presents basic data for the DCD and TDC groups. Electromagnetic motion-tracking device A Polhemus Fastrak (Polhemus Inc., Colchester, VT, USA) was used to record the 3D position and orientation of each participant’s dominant upper limb in all experimental condi- tions. Sensors were attached to: (i) the dorsal side (nail) of the index finger, (ii) the midpoint of the third dorsal metacarpal, (iii) the styloid process of the ulna, and (iv) the lateral epicondyle of the humerus, to capture the movement of the index finger, palm, forearm, and upper arm, separately. The transmitter was fitted to a 100-cm high plastic stand, located 30 cm behind the force plate. All kinematic data were recorded at 30 Hz. Assessment of sensitivity to light touch Sensitivity to LT on the dominant index fingertip was evaluated using von Frey filaments, with forces ranging from 0.008 to