Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 51-3 | Page 60

A. Morral et al. 10.00 9.00 8.00 206 7.00 6.00 5.00 4.00 3.00 2.00 1.00 0.00 Basal 1 month Group I- Standard device 2 months 4 months Group II- Sophisticated Device 14 months Group III- Austere device Fig. 6. Visual analogue scale (VAS) assessment of pain during the day, over time and by device group. Time factor p  < 0.001; device factor p  = 0.853; device–time interaction factor p  = 0.599. 8.00 7.00 6.00 5.00 4.00 3.00 2.00 1.00 0.00 Basal Group I- Standard device 4 months Time Group II- Sophisticated Device 14 months Group III- Austere device Fig. 7. Fascia thickness over time and by device group. Time factor p  < 0.001; device factor p  = 0.402; device–time interaction factor p  = 0.800. Therapeutic context includes a multitude of signals inherent to any intervention, which are perceived and interpreted by patients and generate positive or nega- tive expectations (12). Placebo and nocebo effects can be generated from expectations (27–29); therefore it is important to assess contextual factors that can generate expectations and their impact on clinical outcomes. This RCT was designed to evaluate the influence of the appearance of a physical agent; in this case a rESW device, on clinical outcomes in plantar fasciitis. It was found that, in the treatment of chronic plantar fasciitis with rESW, the appearance of the device did not significantly affect the clinical results. None of the 5 clinical variables analysed (foot function, pain with the first weight-bearing step in the morning, pain during the day, plantar fascia thickness, and adverse effects) showed statistically significant differences bet- ween the 3 groups: patients had similar results regard- less of the treatment device (standard, sophisticated, www.medicaljournals.se/jrm or austere device). Furthermore, all patients improved significantly over time in relation to the baseline assess- ment. The improvement was observed equally in all 3 groups and at all assessment time-points: 1, 2, 4 and 14 months after the last rESWT session, with no statisti- cally significant differences among groups over time. That is, the clinical outcomes were independent of the device used. Although the appearance of the device neither improved nor worsened treatment outcomes, as shown in this study, small differences were observed between the austere group and the other groups. The austere group obtained, in most evaluations, the worst clinical outcomes. Despite an extensive neuroscience base that supports the placebo effect, there is a lack of clinical research that explores, in a healthcare setting, the context and placebo responses that accompany the overall thera- peutic intervention. Most existing knowledge about the placebo effect has come from studies in basic science and clinical trials that are far removed from usual clini- cal practice. However, it has been found that placebo responses are greater under these conditions than in clinical trials conducted in real healthcare settings (21). Few clinical trials have analysed treatment context factors, such as the appearance of a physical agent and its possible influence on patient recovery, making it difficult to compare the results of the current study with those obtained in similar studies. In a study con- ducted by Dawes et al. (30), 2 identical hearing aids were compared: 1 described as “new” and the other as “conventional”. Approximately 75% of the participants preferred the new device and reported that the hearing quality it provided was superior to the conventional one (30). Another clinical trial, with real patients, investigated the influence of different verbal infor- mation combined with a real analgesic drug (31) and reported similar results to the present study, showing no differences in pain reduction. Furthermore, in a cros- sover clinical trial, it was shown that the same person may respond differently to different types of placebo (placebo tablet or sham acupuncture). The response to placebo is a complex phenomenon that has many variables and goes beyond patient characteristics. This could explain the difficulty of detecting a pattern for people “responding to placebo” (32, 33). The magnitude of the placebo effect depends on numerous design factors. A 2015 meta-analysis found that the type of active drug (opioid or non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug (NSAID)), the scheduled follow-up visits (number of planned face-to-face vi- sits), and randomization ratio (probability of receiving a placebo treatment, 1:4 or 1:1) were predictive of the magnitude of the placebo response, thereby supporting the expectancy hypothesis. Exploratory models sho-