Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 51-1CompleteIssue | Page 25

22 M. L. M. Wijenberg et al. Table V. Factor loadings based on exploratory factor analysis after Oblimin rotation Factor 1 “Maladaptive PF” b Factor 2 ”Adaptive PF” Proactive coping Two years post-stroke a Two months post-stroke Self-efficacy c Extraversion Optimism –0.36 0.72 c Passive coping Neuroticism Pessimism Eigenvalues Explained variance, % Factor correlation Factor 1 ”Adaptive PF” 0.61 0.81 0.85 0.77 0.37 0.33 0.58 Factor 2 ”Maladaptive PF” –0.51 0.68 0.86 3.03 1.02 43.55 –0.58 0.97 0.35 0.86 3.76 54.84 –0.70 Factor loadings < 0.3 are suppressed. a Forced 2-factor analysis. b Factor correlation and loadings are inverted for interpretation purposes. c Based at 2 months post-stroke on transformed data due to non-normality. PF: psychological factor. psychological factor”, respectively. Passive coping and neuroticism had positive loadings on the maladaptive factor, whereas optimism had a negative loading on this factor. Proactive coping, self-efficacy, extraversion and optimism had positive loadings on the adaptive factor. Pessimism did not load on any factor. The explained variance of the 2 factors together was 43.6%. At 2 years post-stroke, factors 1 and 2 are labelled as “adaptive psychological factor” and “maladaptive psychological factor”, respectively. Proactive coping, self-efficacy and extraversion had positive loadings on the adaptive factor. Passive coping, neuroticism and pessimism had positive loadings on the maladaptive factor. Furthermore, optimism had a negative loading on this factor. The explained variance of the 2 factors together was 54.8%. DISCUSSION This study found that scores on measures of psycho- logical factors changed during the first 2 years post- stroke. The psychological factors were moderately to strongly correlated with each other and over time and clustered at both time-points into 2 factors: an “adap- tive psychological factor” (proactive coping, self-effi- cacy, extraversion) and a “maladaptive psychological factor” (passive coping and neuroticism). Scores on all adaptive psychological factors decreased over time, whereas scores on maladaptive psychological factors increased or remained stable (passive coping) over time. The added value of our study is that we evaluated the temporal stability of multiple psychological factors simultaneously in a large cohort of patients with stroke using a longitudinal design. To the best of our knowledge, the finding of a nega- tive temporal impact across all psychological factors post-stroke has not been demonstrated previously. As mentioned in the introduction, previous research on www.medicaljournals.se/jrm temporal stability of psychological factors in stroke and other populations showed inconsistent results across and within psychological factors, possibly due to the use of different measures, time-points and designs. Strikingly, even though the sample consisted mainly of patients with a mild stroke overall changes in a ne- gative direction were found. In comparing our results with previous findings regarding temporal stability of psychological factors in healthy populations, we found different patterns of changes over time. Most studies assessing psychological factors in healthy adults showed temporal stability (9–14), whereas our data showed temporal changes in a negative direction across all psychological factors. This suggests that the occurrence of a stroke could be a possible cause of the observed negative changes over time. The occurrence of stroke results in negative changes regarding emotional, cognitive and energy resources, which could provide an explanation for the observed negative change of psychological factors over time. After a stroke, many patients suffer from depression (30), cognitive complaints (31) and disabling fatigue (32), increasing their burden and calling on their reserves of resilience. The observed negative chan- ges in psychological factors could be concomitant with these negative emotional, cognitive and energy changes. In fact, it was shown that neuroticism is concomitant with depressive symptoms (33), and that cognitive complaints in patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) are associated with the use of ma- ladaptive coping styles (7). In our sample, 66% were considered to be cognitively impaired (MoCA score ≤ 25) at 2 months post-stroke. Furthermore, Wu et al. (32) proposed a biopsychosocial model including psychological factors as an explanation for fatigue after stroke. Taken together, stroke is associated with negative changes in emotional, cognitive or energy resources. These consequences of stroke may form