iHerp Australia Issue 12 | Page 13

Thankfully, Neville Burns is not one to hold a grudge. Despite nearly losing his life to a brown snake bite, he believes they suffer from a poor public image, and talks about the species with something akin to affection! I n the eyes of the public, there is, it seems, no other venomous Australian snake that deserves as much negative feedback as this species. Comments such as 'very aggressive’, 'nasty snakes', and 'will chase you' are often heard. The fact that the Eastern Brown Snake ranks as number one for human deaths amongst Australian snakes only helps to contribute to its fearsome reputation and the exaggerated claims of unprovoked attacks. Based on the LD50 test used by scientists to determine the toxicity of venom (i.e. the lethal dose of venom that kills 50% of laboratory mice) the Eastern Brown Snake is the second most venomous snake in the world - outranked only by another Australian species, the Inland Taipan. The lower the LD50 the more lethal the venom, and the Eastern Brown Snake’s rating is 0.053mg/kg (milligrams of venom per kilogram of body weight). However, many herpetologists question if this can be accurately applied to humans as human physiology is far different to that of mice. Studies show a marked change in the venom of the Eastern Brown Snake as the snake matures. Juvenile specimens feed mainly on lizards and their venom is chiefly neurotoxic in nature, but the venom of more mature specimens, which feed largely on rats and mice, acts primarily through powerful procoagulants. The exact age at which this change occurs is yet to be determined. As a full-time snake exhibitor and training course provider I am often asked by members of the public which species I consider the most dangerous to handle. And yes, in my opinion the Eastern Brown Snake is one of the most dangerous snakes to handle, as it is very agile and will defend itself vigorously. But does this mean it deserves its reputation for aggression and ‘chasing’ people? Not at all! Eastern Brown Snakes, like any other venomous snakes, prefer to use their considerable speed to avoid confrontations with humans rather than attack. Often the snake will remain still when approached, in the hope that it is not detected due to its cryptic colouration. Snakes lack the mental capacity to weigh up situations analytically, and react instinctively. Venom is for two purposes only; to kill prey items and to aid in digestion. No human looks like prey to a venomous snake. And even if a snake attacked by a large predator (or human) is able to deliver a meaningful bite, by the time the venom takes effect, it is likely that snake itself is injured or dead. It makes more sense, therefore, for the snake to either flee, or bluff by appearing to be willing to strike. A landmark study by Rick Shine and Pat Whitaker, published in 1999, indicated quite clearly that the Eastern Brown Snake’s reputation for aggression is undeserved. Personally, I hate the term ‘aggression’ being applied to snakes, as it is my experience that snakes are merely defensive. Certainly, Eastern Brown Snakes can be dangerous and difficult if ‘tailed’ or otherwise handled, but the reptile cannot be denied the right to defend itself. Furthermore, the average member of the public has little understand- ing of a snake’s reactions and responses and can often misconstrue defensive behaviour as aggression. The site for Shine and Whitaker’s study at Yanco in New South Wales was chosen on the basis of the number of shed skins in the area, and was about one kilometre away from an escape channel I had ‘The Eastern Brown Snake is one of the MOST DANGEROUS snakes to handle; but does this mean that it deserves its reputation for AGGRESSION ?’ recommended because of the number of Eastern Brown Snakes I had encountered at this location – and the number collected by local snake man Roy Reynolds, who for years had supplied Eric Worrell and the Cann brothers. In this study, the research- ers had 455 encounters with Eastern Brown Snakes, and found that the snakes were far more tolerant than was popularly believed. Less than 3% of the snakes actually advanced towards the scientists; 40% retreated, while 40% remained stationary, and 9% ignored the human. Moving snakes were more likely to react, with those that were stationary relying on lack of movement and camouflage to avoid detection. The researchers also discovered that snakes were more likely to recognise the approach of a person wearing dark clothing and move away, whereas a slow approach or cloud cover increased the risk of a close encounter. Of the snakes that did react, 58% gave what was termed a ‘full’ warning, with the forebody raised high off the ground, the neck in an ‘S’ shape, and the mouth sometimes open. From this position strikes are slower, but delivered with greater accuracy, and are more likely to result in a meaningful bite with