iHerp Australia Issue 11 | Page 9

release small Cane Toads just in advance of the invasion front. It’s easy to find sites where invasion is bound to occur within a few months. Predators such as goannas eat those small toads, get sick, and avoid toads thereafter. So when the invasion front arrives, those educated predators don’t try to eat the (large) toads in the invasion vanguard; they already know that toads make you nauseous. As a result, the predators survive. But what happens next? Unlike quolls, reptiles don't teach their young how to forage, so mum can’t teach the babies to avoid Cane Toads. Does that mean that training predators is a waste of time - saving one generation, only to have their offspring die instead? No. Within a year or two of arriving in a site, the toads begin to breed - producing hordes of tiny amphibians that can serve as perfect ‘teachers’ for the offspring of any predators that survived the initial onslaught. One generation of education is all we need to blunt the impact of the toad invasion. Some luckless individual may still die if the first toad it encounters is too big. But this loss is more than offset if the predator’s brothers and sisters survive long enough to reproduce. A few years ago, when I suggested releasing small toads in advance of the main toad invasion, the general reaction was negative. And that’s putting it mildly. People in the path of the toad invasion thought that this was quite the most stupid idea they had ever heard. Except, they didn’t phrase it quite as politely as that. But things changed when we did the experiments to test the concept. We trained young quolls not to eat toads, and those quolls survived. Tragically, their untrained siblings died as soon as they encountered their first toad. We trained blue-tongue skinks not to eat toads, and they survived. Other skinks, treated the same way except for the training, were all dead within a few weeks of toad arrival. Yellow-spotted Goannas that we trained to avoid toads survived, despite being surrounded by toads a few months later, whereas untrained goannas died. Again and again, the idea worked. And so, opinion shifted. More and more people decided it was worth a try. Most importantly, my student Georgia Ward-Fear worked collaboratively with Aboriginal groups in implementing the method, building strong support from the traditional owners of important sites in the Kimberley. So, it’s happening. An idea that sounded like lunacy (“WTF!!! some southern boffin wants to release MORE bloody Cane Toads!!??”) won across-the-board support, and we are working with a coalition of conservation groups to implement Conditioned Taste Aversion